• Log in with Facebook Log in with Twitter Log In with Google      Sign In    
  • Create Account
  LongeCity
              Advocacy & Research for Unlimited Lifespans

Photo
* * * * * 1 votes

loneliness in borderline schizotypy

outcome treatment strategy

  • Please log in to reply
14 replies to this topic

#1 Jedi_Nights

  • Guest
  • 5 posts
  • 7
  • Location:MI
  • NO

Posted 14 October 2016 - 01:57 AM


I've done dialectical therapy, risperidone, meetup.com, and okcupid.  You might say, "well jeez mate, you've done it all.  Surely things have gotten better?"  A bit, but in other respects, I've come out a little worse for wear, even regressed.  Though there's truthfully one thing I haven't tried.  That's being patient (and consistent / true to the system).  I'm very forgetful, impatient, faithless, distractable, amorphous, tactless, indecisive and non-committal.. some would say boring, un-advenurous or -spontaneous (but maybe they're just going for charming or dreamy, the underhanded way).

 

My inconsistent desire (ability?) to go out or party is at odds with my consistent desire to improve socially.  An ongoing identity crisis, at all levels: professional, personal and romantic.  People are always telling me to get my shit together.  I never make much money or have a want to travel.  I would like to think I'm a smart guy, but I've made so many mistakes over the years that it seems unlikely.

 

I've gotten better at keeping my head up and being "cooler" around people, which I'm attributing to exposure, meditation/self-therapy, yoga, and a shift to cooking at home.  I try not to make obvious (at least until I put in my two weeks') my turbulent and essentially solitary inner-life, my frustration, peculiarity and emptiness.  Keyword: try.  Sort of deleted my facebook to escape the judgment of my peers.  Though I miss it, I find myself filling the void by making cringe conversation elsewhere, off the grid.  Over about a year and a half of active work & due diligence, it seems I've only made a 40 per cent recovery.  A long road ahead.

 

Here's the best (an only) representation of borderline I took the time to watch.

 

As for a representation of schizotypy.. I stumbled on this creature, a severe case.  He has a more romantic, literary, sentimental poetry slam.

 

Here's a more moderate case, with a very slight borderline element:

 

Most importantly you can tell by the pretentiousness of their vernacular, dudes aint getting laid.

so if you got funny openers, shoot your boy a message

 


  • like x 1

#2 PeaceAndProsperity

  • Guest
  • 1,194 posts
  • -195
  • Location:Heaven

Posted 14 October 2016 - 06:38 PM

So let me get this straight, you don't have social contact and you are feeling lonely?



sponsored ad

  • Advert
Advertisements help to support the work of this non-profit organisation. To go ad-free join as a Member.

#3 gamesguru

  • Guest
  • 3,467 posts
  • 429
  • Location:coffeelake.intel.int

Posted 16 October 2016 - 09:55 AM

Sounds like hehas professional problems too.

I'm in a similar boat.. never really had friends. Also have professional problems, never very motivated, able to stick to something for very long, or function at expert level even w training. I did well in high school until 2011, and had at least some connection with my classmates, but since 2012 or so, the diseases seem to be consuming me. Dropped out of uni in 2014 without making a friend in 3yrs. The only ppl I keep on with are parents and one neighbor, but these are admittedly not stable relationships. I don't know if it's me or maybe my methods.. making small talk is alright but things fall apart over the course of getting to know someone. A lot of anxiety and negative emotions around people, and it's hard to come up with jokes

Edited by gamesguru, 16 October 2016 - 09:58 AM.

  • Well Written x 1

#4 PeaceAndProsperity

  • Guest
  • 1,194 posts
  • -195
  • Location:Heaven

Posted 16 October 2016 - 10:57 AM

I haven't had any friends since I was around 10, that's more than 12 years ago. 

My problem with forming friendships is the fact of being socially withdrawn, not feeling any desire for socializing. Oddly I also don't feel a need for sexual partners. Add in the feeling of being uncomfortable in the presence of others and it only gets worse.

Perhaps if we found what receptor or whatever it could be, is implicated in social withdrawal, we'd find the cause of the problem.

 



#5 gamesguru

  • Guest
  • 3,467 posts
  • 429
  • Location:coffeelake.intel.int

Posted 16 October 2016 - 04:03 PM

That sounds like schizoid on first sight,because schizotypals have a need for a social life and not having it causes great frustration but the schizoid has a meh, take it or leave it attitude.

But when i get in a group or party setting, the social anhedonia becomes huge introversion factor, with only anxiety being bigger.

I think it's mostly glutamate and dopamine, problem is very deeply ingrained and site specific, so hard to treat herbally. Stuff like exercise and NAC is supposed to help with negative symptoms, but it's been no saving grace.

Have you had romantic partners since 12 lol?

Maybe just need to accept impaired functioning and your lot in life (omega male), and not get too angry about it?

#6 PeaceAndProsperity

  • Guest
  • 1,194 posts
  • -195
  • Location:Heaven

Posted 16 October 2016 - 04:41 PM

That sounds like schizoid on first sight,because schizotypals have a need for a social life and not having it causes great frustration but the schizoid has a meh, take it or leave it attitude.

But when i get in a group or party setting, the social anhedonia becomes huge introversion factor, with only anxiety being bigger.

I think it's mostly glutamate and dopamine, problem is very deeply ingrained and site specific, so hard to treat herbally. Stuff like exercise and NAC is supposed to help with negative symptoms, but it's been no saving grace.

Have you had romantic partners since 12 lol?

Maybe just need to accept impaired functioning and your lot in life (omega male), and not get too angry about it?

No, as far as I know, schizotypy is characterized by a reduced or lack of desire for socializing as well as all the other stuff like extreme social anxiety and so on.

What does social anhedonia mean to you? The word "anhedonia" is a tricky one in literature since it means tens of things.

What do you think it is with glutamate and dopamine, underactivity or overactivity at certain receptors?

 

"Have you had romantic partners since 12 lol?" Was this meant for me? No, I've never had a romantic partner and to some extent I'm not interested in it either. Why would you ask this?

"Maybe just need to accept impaired functioning and your lot in life (omega male), and not get too angry about it?" I assume this is meant for the OP.

The whole "omega, beta, alpha male" is MGTOW nonsense for effeminate male college students who don't understand how male behavior or the social structure works. There's no such thing as an "alpha male" or "beta male" or whatever else. There are masculine men and they behave in a certain way, and feminine men who behave in a certain way, and then there are men with odd combinations of personality traits.

As for being an "omega" in the sense of being submissive or being a reject by most women, this is a total misunderstanding of the sexual roles. It's supposed to be the male who picks his sexual partner based upon her attractiveness and she has zero to say in it. We live in times where women prefer being a man and many men prefer being women, but this goes back thousands of years I bet. Arabic-Islamic society is a perfect example of a healthy social structure, there's no "beta alpha omega" crap.



#7 gamesguru

  • Guest
  • 3,467 posts
  • 429
  • Location:coffeelake.intel.int

Posted 16 October 2016 - 06:04 PM

It sounds like schizoid.  Consider yourself lucky not wanting romance.  You're like a monk, you just eat your fish raw.  I'm over here rubbing two sticks together, desperately trying to get a fire going.

 

And yeah, that was for the OP.  If you're not getting frustrated by your loneliness, you're not in the same boat as us.

 

Okay, but I don't think your distinction of feminine and masculine men is what is needed here, because plenty of effeminate types do better than some bitter, tooth-grinding masculine types.  Yeah but a man can't pick up on a partner as long as he's dealing with constant rejection.  I think islam is very polygamous and misogynistic, almost a rape-state, I wouldn't call it healthy or balanced.  I don't think choosing a mate based on violence, arraigned marriage or heavy financial blackmail is a "balanced" way to pick a mate.  That's just not how I picture it.. your character and sense of humor are supposed to play a major role.  And then, that guy who can't consistently demonstrate wit or a winning personality becomes the omega male.  I don't think it's very descriptive calling him a bitch, just because he has identity, communication, emotional problems.  He just has problems relating and expressing himself.

 

Social anhedonia just means not perceiving a joy or meaning in interactions.  Like everyone is laughing at a joke, but why?  Someone laughed at mine, so what?  Someone has some hot gossip or a cute story to tell, who cares, what's it to me?  It's general indifference in the face of society, not getting excited about the same little things, being flat, bored, un-emotive.  But it's double-sided, because you find yourself drawn to these situations, like everyone's going to party, I should go along otherwise they'll think I hate them and probably not invite me again, but I get to the party and the whole atmosphere is suddenly one big "meh, I wanna go home".  I guess it comes also in the form of motivational anhedonia, but primarily decisional or consummatory anhedonia.  The motivational aspect or "wanting" is relatively intact, or at least remains so with self-optimism (e.g. i need to go to this party), but the consummatory aspect or "liking" is severely crippled (i'm not having fun at this party), as is the decisional aspect (which is like the ego, a mediator between immediate wants and experience of prior liking... e.g. not sure if i'll be going to a party again).

 

As far as I know, schizotypy is characterized by a normal desire for interaction but a reduce ability for it.

 

There are many similarities between the schizotypal and schizoid personalities. Most notable of the similarities is the inability to initiate or maintain relationships (both friendly and romantic). The difference between the two seems to be that those labeled as schizotypal avoid social interaction because of a deep-seated fear of people. The schizoid individuals simply feel no desire to form relationships, because they see no point in sharing their time with others.

 

I definitely have trust issues, but I'm also toying with the idea of avoidant personality.  I feel like there's a lot of overlap between schizotypal, avoidant, and plain social anxiety.  It's a mess, because people who meet borderline criteria sometimes meet criteria for 4 or 5 different personality disorders.  You could be vaguely antisocial, schizoid, schizotypal, avoidant, histrionic, AND borderline.

 

People with this disorder are hypersensitive to rejection. Because of  this, they avoid situations with any possible conflict. This reaction is fear-driven. People with avoidant personality disorder become disturbed by their own social isolation, withdrawal, and inability to form close, interpersonal relationships.


  • Agree x 2
  • WellResearched x 1

#8 airplanepeanuts

  • Guest
  • 352 posts
  • 15
  • Location:Earth

Posted 16 October 2016 - 10:13 PM

 

Social anhedonia just means not perceiving a joy or meaning in interactions.  Like everyone is laughing at a joke, but why?  Someone laughed at mine, so what?  Someone has some hot gossip or a cute story to tell, who cares, what's it to me?  It's general indifference in the face of society, not getting excited about the same little things, being flat, bored, un-emotive.  But it's double-sided, because you find yourself drawn to these situations, like everyone's going to party, I should go along otherwise they'll think I hate them and probably not invite me again, but I get to the party and the whole atmosphere is suddenly one big "meh, I wanna go home".

 

This is just the worst thing because humans are a super social species. Basically it'll make you feel like an outcast.



#9 PeaceAndProsperity

  • Guest
  • 1,194 posts
  • -195
  • Location:Heaven

Posted 17 October 2016 - 06:05 AM

humans are a super social species.

That's absolutely false. People today are hypersocial because of genetic mutations. People in the past clearly weren't hypersocial. In all the African tribes studied, none were hypersocial.

As people's intelligence have been increasing due to the accumulation of genetic mutations, so it is with the social tendencies. 


  • Needs references x 2

#10 gamesguru

  • Guest
  • 3,467 posts
  • 429
  • Location:coffeelake.intel.int

Posted 17 October 2016 - 10:50 AM

Yeah, but no one wants to be around an awkward introvert, so what to do, take better tinder pics, or maybe just call people, invite yourself, keep your mouth shut more, or be careful how you present yourself? But even that I would barely know where to begin. Very difficult for me.

What's absolutely false is that humans have undergone significant evolution in the past 10000 years. We're more social today because we have tools like facebook and smartphones at our fingertips, and because we have more free time to celebrate.. not because of some mutations we recently acquired. Leave it to the schizoid perspective to come up with this. And people 10000 years ago were just as smart, that's when mud architecture started, and not long after that, philosophy.
  • Enjoying the show x 1

#11 PeaceAndProsperity

  • Guest
  • 1,194 posts
  • -195
  • Location:Heaven

Posted 17 October 2016 - 11:22 AM

Schizo-this, schizo-that. It's all in my head, a screw gone loose. I need to be heavily medicated to get me to lose the wrong kind of ideas. I think that most liberal social reforms are bad. WHAT? Am I a SCHIZOphrenic or something! Everybody knows that everything is a social construct and nothing exists! Gosh!

 

By "hypersocial" I mean that the person is more likely to adopt new ways of speaking and thinking. Men in Hollywood are an example of this. You can literally make these people, most people, believe anything, there are no limits. Being hypersocial is sort of the same as being feminine.

It's also a widely accepted "scientific fact" that people are more cooperative today and one of the explanations for this are world-wide declining levels of androgens. Whatever the explanation is, it really is just an extension of being hypersocial.

 

Btw I wouldn't mind being able to function socially and desiring to be in the presence of others, what I just don't want is to turn into a half-woman, an effeminate male who wants to par-tah every single day, consume alcohol and mindless drugs and "have a good time," my brain just has too many connections to fall to that teeny level.

I also have "social anhedonia" including "sexual anhedonia".

 

Maybe you should use your great study-finding and study-reading skills to find out the models in literature for social withdrawal and dysfunction in autism and schizophrenia, and perhaps then we can test and find the cause of our deficits. I too have been testing on myself and I find that SJW can 1/10 times induce full social desire with better social functioning but when it doesn't work it causes anxiety and other issues.



#12 gamesguru

  • Guest
  • 3,467 posts
  • 429
  • Location:coffeelake.intel.int

Posted 17 October 2016 - 10:28 PM

Maybe you should use your great study-finding and study-reading skills to find out the models in literature for social withdrawal and dysfunction in autism and schizophrenia, and perhaps then we can test and find the cause of our deficits.

 

Hey man.  Not cool.  If I want to wallow in negativity, let me.  Don't go bursting my bubble like that.  But in all seriousness, this has triggered a light-bulb, or a train, in my head.  +Rep.  I'm curious to see how much of this applies to you, and whether you could make it apply, or whether you want to do thing about your situation, after reading the bits in bold.

 

The problem with making a model and finding a solution is that the correlates of loneliness aren't understood, all we know is SSRIs, oxytocin, allopregnanolone are mildly effective.  We don't know if it's because of serotonin or c-Fos or what.  But you can start making inferences, for example: forsoklin boosts oxytocin, and might therefore be expected to alleviate loneliness.. bacopa is a weak SSRI, so it might also help.. and so on, until we find our way in the dark.

 

Front Psychol. 2016 Jul 7;7:1018. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01018. eCollection 2016.

Loneliness and Schizotypy Are Distinct Constructs, Separate from General Psychopathology.

Badcock JC1, Barkus E2, Cohen AS3, Bucks R4, Badcock DR4.

 

Abstract

Loneliness is common in youth and associated with a significantly increased risk of psychological disorders. Although loneliness is strongly associated with psychosis, its relationship with psychosis proneness is unclear. Our aim in this paper was to test the hypothesis that loneliness and schizotypal traits, conveying risk for schizophrenia spectrum disorders, are similar but separate constructs. Pooling data from two non-clinical student samples (N = 551) we modeled the structure of the relationship between loneliness and trait schizotypy. Loneliness was assessed with the University of California, Los Angeles Loneliness Scale (UCLA-3), whilst negative (Social Anhedonia) and positive (Perceptual Aberrations) schizotypal traits were assessed with the Wisconsin Schizotypy Scales-Brief (WSS-B). Fit statistics indicated that the best fitting model of UCLA-3 scores comprises three correlated factors (Isolation, Related Connectedness, and Collective Connectedness), consistent with previous reports. Fit statistics for a two factor model of positive and negative schizotypy were excellent. Next, bi-factor analysis was used to model a general psychopatholgy factor (p) across the three loneliness factors and separate negative and positive schizotypy traits. The results showed that all items (except 1) co-loaded on p. However, with the influence of p removed, additional variance remained within separate sub-factors, indicating that loneliness and negative and positive trait schizotypy are distinct and separable constructs. Similarly, once shared variance with p was removed, correlations between sub-factors of loneliness and schizotypal traits were non-significant. These findings have important clinical implications since they suggest that loneliness should not be conflated with the expression of schizotypy. Rather, loneliness needs to be specifically targeted for assessment and treatment in youth at risk for psychosis.

 

Perspect Psychol Sci. 2015 Mar; 10(2): 238–249.

Loneliness: Clinical Import and Interventions

Stephanie Cacioppo,1,2Angela J. Grippo,3Sarah London,2,4Luc Goossens,5 and John T. Cacioppo2,4

 Abstract 

In 1978, when the Task Panel report to the US President’s Commission on Mental Health emphasized the importance of improving health care and easing the pain of those suffering from emotional distress syndromes including loneliness, few anticipated that this issue would still need to be addressed 40 years later. A meta-analysis (Masi et al., 2011) on the efficacy of treatments to reduce loneliness identified a need for well-controlled randomized clinical trials focusing on the rehabilitation of maladaptive social cognition. We review assessments of loneliness and build on this meta-analysis to discuss the efficacy of various treatments for loneliness. With the advances made over the past 5 years in the identification of the psychobiological and pharmaceutical mechanisms associated with loneliness and maladaptive social cognition, there is increasing evidence for the potential efficacy of integrated interventions that combine (social) cognitive behavioral therapy with short-term adjunctive pharmacological treatments.

Life in America in the 21st century is unlike any period in human history. People are living longer than ever before, and the rise in the Internet has transformed how Americans work, play, search, shop, study, communicate, and relate to one another. People are increasingly connected digitally, but the prevalence of loneliness (perceived social isolation) also appears to be rising. From a prevalence estimated to be 11–17% in the 1970s (Peplau, Russell, & Heim, 1979), loneliness has increased to over 40% in middle aged and older adults1 (Edmondson, 2010; Perissinotto, Cenzer, & Covinsky, 2012). Over the past 40 years, loneliness has also become more widespread overseas (e.g., Victor, Scambler, Bowling, & Bond, 2005; Randall, 2012; Victor & Yang, 2012; Stickley et al., 2013). For instance, a 2010 survey from Statistics New Zealand shows that 33% of individuals aged 15 and above experienced loneliness in the four weeks preceding the survey. In the U.K., prevalence of loneliness is estimated between 5%–6% (for individuals reporting feeling “often” lonely), 21%–31% (for individuals reporting feeling “sometimes” lonely; Victor et al., 2005; Victor & Yang, 2012), and prevalence rates as high as 45% have been reported throughout the U.K. according to an online survey that took place in March 2010 (Griffin, 2010). As the prevalence of loneliness rises, evidence accrues that loneliness is a major risk factor for poor physical and mental health outcomes.

 Definition of Loneliness 

Psychiatrist Fromm-Reichmann (1959) raised awareness of loneliness and noted the need for a rigorous, scientific definition of loneliness. In the decades that followed, loneliness as a psychological condition was characterized, and measures for quantifying individual differences were introduced (e.g., Lynch & Convey, 1979; Peplau, Russell, & Heim, 1979; Russell, Peplau, & Cutrona, 1980; Weiss, 1973). Loneliness corresponds to a discrepancy between an individual’s preferred and actual social relations (Peplau & Perlman, 1982). This discrepancy then leads to the negative experience of feeling alone and/or the distress and dysphoria of feeling socially isolated even when among family or friends (Weiss, 1973). This definition underscores the fact that feeling alone or lonely does not necessarily mean being alone nor does being alone necessarily mean feeling alone (see J. T. Cacioppo et al., this issue). One can feel lonely in the crowd or in a marriage. Reciprocally, one may enjoy being alone (a pleasant state defined as solitude; Tillich, 1959) at times in order to reach personal growth experiences (such as those achieved through solitary meditation or mindfulness exercises) or to simply take a temporary break from dealing with the demands of modern life.

Loneliness emphasizes the fact that social species require not simply the presence of others but also the presence of significant others whom they can trust, who give them a goal in life, with whom they can plan, interact, and work together to survive and prosper (J. T. Cacioppo & Patrick, 2008). Moreover, the physical presence of significant others in one’s social environment is not a sufficient condition. One needs to feel connected to significant others to not feel lonely. Accordingly, one can be temporarily alone and not feel lonely as they feel highly connected with their spouse, family, and/or friends – even at a distance. Subjectivity and perception of the friendly or hostile nature of one’s social environment is, thus, a characteristic of loneliness. As comedian Robin Williams said: “I used to think the worst thing in life was to end up all alone. It’s not. The worst thing in life is to end up with people who make you feel all alone” (2009). Although this crucial component of loneliness helps better differentiate subjective social isolation (loneliness) from objective social isolation, it has led occasionally to a conflation of loneliness and other dysphoric states (e.g., social anxiety, depression) in which a person’s subjective experiencing of their social environment plays also a crucial role.

A main challenge for physicians and mental health clinicians has been, therefore, to become sufficiently informed about the scientific definition of loneliness so that other mental disorders were not mistakenly diagnosed and treated when loneliness was either the primary presenting problem or the cause of the depression for which treatment was sought (Booth, 2000). For instance, because loneliness and depression share some characteristics and a correlation ranging from .38 to .71 (cf., Booth, 2000; J. T. Cacioppo et al., 2006), many clinicians believed, for decades, that loneliness was simply an aspect of depression with no distinct concept worthy of study (cf. Young, 1982). There is now considerable evidence showing that loneliness and depression are separable and that loneliness increases the risk for depression (J. T. Cacioppo et al., 2006; Heinrich & Gullone, 2006). In 1980, for instance, Weeks et al. administered loneliness and depression scales to undergraduate college students. Using data from 333 subjects, they concluded that loneliness and depression, though correlated with each other, were “clearly different constructs.” These results have been replicated and extended in recent longitudinal research (J. T. Cacioppo et al., 2010; VanderWeele et al., 2011), and reinforced Ostrov and Offer’s (1978) clinical observation that a potential difference between loneliness and depression was that while both are filled with helplessness and pain, loneliness is characterized by the hope that all would be perfect if only the lonely person could be united with another longed for person.

Go to:

 Who Can Feel Lonely? 

Loneliness does not discriminate. Although loneliness is more likely in populations who are at risk for social alienation, isolation, and separation (such as older individuals who live alone and are isolated from friends and families; patients with psychiatric disorders; patients with long-term health conditions that limit their communicative capacities or mobility; and/or stigmatized groups; for reviews see Andersson, 1998, J. T. Cacioppo in this special issue), anyone can feel lonely at any time. For instance, the same objective social relationship can be perceived as caring and protective or as exploitive and isolating based on a host of factors including an individual’s prior experiences, current attributions, and overall preference for social contact (J. T. Cacioppo, Cacioppo, Capitanio, & Cole, 2015). Accordingly, loneliness is not limited to older adults who are infirmed and live alone but is evident across the lifespan and is even evident in people surrounded by large numbers of other young adults or with numerous contacts/followers/friends on social media (e.g., Qualter et al., this issue).

Go to:

 Dimensions of Loneliness 

Loneliness is a complex construct that includes three related facets or dimensions: 1) Intimate loneliness; 2) Relational loneliness; and 3) Collective loneliness (Hawkley et al., 2005; Hawkley, Gu, Luo, & Cacioppo, 2012). These three dimensions match the three dimensions surrounding one’s attentional space (Hall, 1963, 1966; Figure 1): intimate space (the closest space surrounding a person), social space (the space in which people feel comfortable interacting with family and acquaintances), and the public space (a more anonymous space). These three dimensions of loneliness also appear to share some correspondence with the structure of human personal social networks identified by Weiss (1973) and Dunbar (2014; Figure 1). The translation of these three dimensions into a structured attentional personal space (Ortigue, Megevand, Perren, Landis, & Blanke, 2006; Ortigue et al., 2003; Rizzolatti et al., 1983, 1987) are interesting in light of analyses of the mental organization for people’s loneliness/social connection (Figure 1). In loneliness, these three dimensions have been found in various populations, such as college students (Hawkley et al., 2005; McWhirter, 1990b) and older adults in the U.S. (Hawkley et al., 2005; Peplau & Perlman, 1982), and in young and older adults in China. (Hawkley et al., 2012)2. Each dimension also corresponds to different types of loneliness.

Figure 1

The three dimensions of loneliness and different compartments of space.

Intimate Loneliness

Intimate loneliness, or what Weiss (1973) termed emotional loneliness, refers to the perceived absence of a significant someone (e.g., a spouse), that is, a person one can rely on for emotional support during crises, who provides mutual assistance, and who affirms one’s value as a person. This form of intimate connection often has considerable self-other overlap (such as that observed between close friends e.g., husband-wife, best-friends; Hall, 1966; Ortigue et al., 2003, 2006). This dimension corresponds to what Dunbar described as the inner core, which can include up to 5 people (the “support clique”) and comprises the people we rely on for emotional support during crises (Dunbar, 2014).

A population-based study of middle-age and older adults showed that the best (negative) predictor of intimate loneliness was marital status, indicating that intimate partners tend to be a primary source of attachment, emotional connection, and emotional support for adults (Hawkley et al., 2005). These results are consistent with several studies indicating that having a significant partner/spouse is associated with lower levels of intimate loneliness and, reciprocally that losing a partner (through divorce or widowhood) is linked to greater intimate loneliness (e.g., J. T. Cacioppo & Patrick, 2008; Hughes et al., 2004; Lopata, Heinemann, & Baum, 1982; Russell, 1982; Waite & Gallagher, 2001; Weiss, 1973).

Relational Loneliness

The second dimension is relational loneliness, or what Weiss (1973) termed social loneliness. It refers to the perceived presence/absence of quality friendships or family connections, that is, connections from the “sympathy group” (Buys & Larson, 1979; Dunbar, 2014) within one’s relational space. According to Dunbar the “sympathy group” can include among 15 and 50 people and comprises core social partners whom we see regularly and from whom we can obtain high-cost instrumental support (e.g. loans, help with projects, child care; Dunbar, 2014).

The relational space is delimitated by the multi-modal (visual, auditory, and tactile) space that permits face-to-face communications and interactions. Like intimate loneliness, social loneliness is found in women as well as men, although there is some evidence that this dimension may tend to play a slightly greater role in influencing loneliness in women than in men (Hawkley et al., 2005). The best (negative) predictor of relational loneliness in middle-aged and older adults is the frequency of contact with significant friends and family, even after statistically controlling for the other two dimensions of loneliness (Hawkley et al., 2005). Nevertheless, it is not the quantity of friends, but the quality of significant friends/confidants that counts (Hawkley et al., 2008). This point is crucial when diagnosing loneliness.

Collective Loneliness

The third dimension is collective loneliness, an aspect that Weiss (1973) did not identify in his qualitative studies. Collective loneliness refers to a person’s valued social identities or “active network” (e.g., group, school, team, or national identity) wherein an individual can connect to similar others at a distance in the collective space. As such, this dimension may correspond to what Dunbar (2014) described as the outermost social layer, which can include among 150 and 1500 people (the “active network”) who can provide with information through weak ties (Granovetter, 1973), as well as low-cost support (Dunbar, 2014). The best (negative) predictor of collective loneliness found in middle-age and older adults was the number of voluntary groups to which individuals belonged: the more voluntary associations to which individuals belonged, the lower their collective loneliness, again even after statistically controlling for the two other dimensions. This dimension of loneliness is found in women as well as men but tends to be slightly more heavily weighted in men than in women (Hawkley et al., 2005). The emergence of a collective dimension of loneliness suggests that we may have evolved the capacity for and motivation to form relationships not only with other individuals but also with groups (e.g., villages or armies), with the consequence being the promotion of social identification and cooperation in adverse conditions (e.g., competition, hunting, or warfare; Brewer, 2004). The identification with and investments in the group, in turn, may increase the likelihood of the continuity of the group, its members, and their individual genetic legacy (J. T. Cacioppo, Cacioppo, & Boomsma, 2014).

 Consequences of Loneliness 

Loneliness can contribute to a constellation of physical and psychiatric dysfunctions and/or psychosocial risk factors, including depressive symptomatology (J. T. Cacioppo et al., 2006; J. T. Cacioppo, Hawkley, & Thisted, 2010; VanderWeele, Hawkley, Thisted, & Cacioppo, 2011), alcoholism (Akerlind & Hornquist, 1992), suicidal thoughts (Rudatsikira, Muula, Siziya, & Twa-Twa, 2007), aggressive behaviors, social anxiety, and impulsivity (e.g., S. Cacioppo, Capitanio, & Cacioppo, 2014; Ernst & Cacioppo, 1999; Kearns et al., 2014). In addition, loneliness is a risk factor for cognitive decline and the progression of Alzheimer’s Disease (Wilson et al., 2007), recurrent stroke (for review see S. Cacioppo, Capitanio, & Cacioppo, 2014), obesity (Lauder, Mummery, Jones, & Caperchione, 2006), increased vascular resistance (J. T. Cacioppo, Hawkley, Crawford et al., 2002), elevated blood pressure (J. T. Cacioppo, Hawkley, Crawford et al., 2002; Hawkley et al., 2006), increased hypothalamic pituitary adrenocortical activity (Adam, Hawkley, Kudielka, & Cacioppo, 2006; Steptoe, Owen, Kunz-Ebrecht, & Brydon, 2004), decreased sleep salubrity (J. T. Cacioppo, Hawkley, Berntson et al., 2002; Pressman et al., 2005), diminished immunity (Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 1984a b; Pressman et al., 2005), an under-expression of genes bearing anti-inflammatory glucocorticoid response elements and an upregulation of pro-inflammatory gene transcripts (Cole et al., 2007, 2011), abnormal ratios of circulating white blood cells (e.g., neutrophils, lymphocytes, and monocytes; Cole, 2008), and premature mortality (e.g., Luo et al., 2012).

 Interventions to Reduce Loneliness 

Increased recognition of loneliness as a risk factor for adverse psychological and physical health outcomes has elevated interest in interventions to reduce chronic loneliness. For instance, the British government is developing several initiatives aiming to improve the life quality and satisfaction of people suffering from the (real or perceived) absence of social relationships (Victor, Scambler, Bowling, & Bond, 2005). Campaigns designed to raise awareness about the growing problem of loneliness and isolation have also been launched in the United Kingdom by five partner organizations (http://www.campaignt...t-the-campaign/), in Denmark by the Crown Princess and her Mary Foundation (http://www.maryfonden.dk/en/loneliness) and the DaneAge Association (Ældre Sagen; http://www.aeldresag...m-ensomhed.aspx), in Canada by the Canadian Seniors Council (http://www.seniorsco.../eng/home.shtml), and in the United States by Oprah Winfrey, Sanjay Gupta, and Gayle King and supported by Skype (http://www.oprah.com...ight-Loneliness), the AARP Foundation Initiative on Social Isolation (http://www.aarp.org/...work/isolation/), and the Do-It Campaign to end isolation (http://women.oshkosh...rg/endisolation). These campaigns are essential to raise awareness about and to reduce the stigma surrounding loneliness, but these represent only a first step. Effective treatments are also needed.

Different types of treatments

To date, there have been a number of attempts to mitigate loneliness. Such interventions include one-on-one interventions (e.g., befriending, Mead et al., 2010; or mentoring, Dickens et al., 2011), group therapy (e.g., groups of lonely people), and wider community interventions (e.g., community events reaching out to a lonely person/s; Cattan et al., 2005; Findlay, 2003; Masi et al., 2011 for reviews). Most of them have been based on the intuitive understanding of loneliness. For instance, a first model has been to provide social support to lonely individuals. That said, as described above, loneliness is not only about getting support, it is also about giving support back and mutual aid.

A second model has been to increase opportunities for social interaction. But, as noted above, a large number of contacts is not equivalent to high quality relationships (Masi et al., 2011). Effects of our own mentation (what we think, what we perceive) involves both conscious and nonconscious mechanisms. Even if lonely individuals want to connect, their non-conscious hypervigilance for social threat can lead them to be negative with or withdraw from others.

A third model to reduce loneliness is based on teaching lonely people to master social skills. For unfortunate individuals who lack of social skills, this may be effective but people are lonely for many reasons other than poor social skills. Experimental research in which loneliness was manipulated shows that most adults have at least minimal social skills, but these adults are more likely to call upon these social skills when they feel low rather than high in loneliness (Cacioppo et al., 2006).

Which type of behavioral intervention is most effective to reduce loneliness?

We addressed this question in a quantitative meta-analysis on the efficacy of the various treatments tested to reduce loneliness between 1970 and 2009 (Masi et al., 2011), and we investigated various moderator variables, such as the experimental design (single group pretest-posttest; nonrandomized comparison group; randomized comparison group) and intervention format (individual or group). Results showed that the mean effect size was much lower when appropriate experimental and statistical controls were implemented, with the effect size for nonrandomized group comparison studies being −.459, and single-group pretest-posttest designs being −.367, whereas the effect size for randomized controlled studies being −.198. This finding implies that if a program or intervention to reduce loneliness is to be evaluated for efficacy – including large-scale programs such as those being introduced in the United States, Canada, Denmark, and the United Kingdom – it is important to control for potential confounding variables (time, expectancy effects, Hawthorne effects, confounding individual differences) to avoid biases that are likely to lead to an overestimate of treatment efficacy.

Contrary to the conclusion of previous narrative reviews carried out since the 1980s, Masi et al.’s (2011) quantitative literature review revealed little evidence for better efficacy of one-to-one individual therapies compared to group therapies. Type of intervention program was a significant moderator, however. Twenty studies met the criteria for randomized group comparison design, and all four primary types of interventions known to reduce loneliness were present in this group. These four primary types of intervention programs were (a) those that increased opportunities for social contact (e.g., social recreation intervention), (b) those that enhanced social support (e.g., through mentoring programs, Buddy-care program, conference calls), © those that focused on social skills (e.g., speaking on the phone, giving and receiving compliments, enhancing nonverbal communication skills), and (d) those that addressed maladaptive social cognition (e.g., cognitive behavioral therapy). Among these four types, interventions designed to address maladaptive social cognition were associated with the largest effect size (mean effect size = −.598).

These studies ranged from one with elderly adults from a nursing home in Tapei who participated in eight weekly sessions designed to increase awareness and expression of their feelings, to identify positive relationships from their past, and to apply these prior experiences to their current relationships (Chiang et al., 2009) to one with high-risk Naval recruits at basic training who met for 45 minutes per week for 9 weeks to learn, discuss, and practice strategies for increasing one’s sense of belonging, decreasing thought distortion, and improving one’s coping and stress management (Williams et al., 2004). Interventions designed to enhance social support produced a significant but small reduction in loneliness (mean effect size = −.162), while interventions to increase opportunities for social interaction (mean effect size = −.062, n.s.) and interventions to improve social skills (mean effect size = −.017, n.s.) were not found to be effective in lowering loneliness. These findings reinforce the notion that interpersonal contact or communication per se is not sufficient to address chronic loneliness in the general population.

One key to (social) cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) in the framework of reducing loneliness is to educate individuals to identify the automatic negative thoughts that they have about others and about social interactions more generally, and to regard these negative thoughts as possibly faulty hypotheses that need to be verified rather than as facts on which to act (Anderson, Horowitz, & French, 1983; McWhirter, 1990a; Young, 1982). By aiming to change maladaptive social perception and cognition (e.g., dysfunctional and irrational beliefs, false attributions, and self-defeating thoughts and interpersonal interactions; Young, 1982; for reviews: Cacioppo & Patrick, 2008; Masi et al., 2011; McWhirter, 1990a), CBT approach implies that loneliness can be decreased (Masi et al., 2011, McWhirter, 1990a, for reviews).

Research on social cognition as a function of loneliness has resulted in the model depicted in Figure 2. According to this model, lonely individuals typically do not voluntarily become lonely; rather, they “find themselves” on one edge of the continuum of social connections (S. Cacioppo & Cacioppo, 2012) and feeling desperately isolated (Booth, 2000). The perception that one is socially on the edge and isolated from others increases the motive for self-preservation. This, then, increases the motivation to connect with others but also increases an implicit hyper-vigilance for social threats, which then can introduce attentional, confirmatory, and memory biases. Given the effects of attention and expectation on anticipated social interactions, behavioral confirmation processes then can incline an individual who feels isolated to have or to place more import on negative social interactions, which if unchecked can reinforce withdrawal, negativity, and feelings of loneliness (e.g., see J. T. Cacioppo & Cacioppo, 2014; J. T. Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2009). This model points to a number of sources of dysfunctional and irrational beliefs, false expectations and attributions, and self-defeating thoughts and interpersonal interactions on which interventions might be designed to operate. For instance, the attentional, confirmatory, and memory biases could be targeted by training in perspective taking, empathy, and identifying automatic negative thoughts about others and about social interactions and in regarding these negative thoughts as possibly faulty hypotheses that need to be verified, whereas faulty behavioral confirmation processes could be targeted by training in mindfulness (Baer, 2003; Creswell et al., 2012) and capitalization (sharing good times; Gable & Reis, 2010; Woods et al., 2014).

Figure 2

The effects of loneliness on social cognition. Modified from J. T. Cacioppo and Hawkley (2009).

In sum, a primary criterion for empirically supported therapies is that they demonstrate efficacy in randomized controlled studies. Although more research is needed, the meta-analysis suggests that interventions designed to modify maladaptive social cognition may be especially worth pursuing. Such interventions can be expensive and time-consuming, and the client’s lack of openness to changing their thoughts about and interactions with others can be an obstacle to effective treatment. It is possible that these interventions may be more effective (or effective for a greater proportion of individuals) if augmented initially by an appropriate pharmacologic treatment.

Potential Adjunctive Pharmacological Treatments

To date, there is no adjunctive pharmacological treatment for loneliness, but animal research sheds promising light on this issue. For instance, research in which a social animal (e.g., prairie voles, titi monkeys) is chronically housed either with a preferred partner or alone has shown that isolation has deleterious neurological (see review by S. Cacioppo, Capitanio, & Cacioppo, 2014) and neuroendocrinological effects (see review by J. T. Cacioppo, Cacioppo, Capitanio, & Cole, 2015). Interestingly, animal research showed that the behavioral effects of social isolation could be improved with pharmacological help. For instance, pharmacological help includes administration of: 1) antidepressants of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) class that have a broad range of effects including (but not restricted to) improving anxiety-like behavior and fear responses (fluoxetine; Pinna, 2010); 2) neurosteroids (such as allopregnanolone, ALLO) that activate the hypothalamic pituitary adrenocortical (HPA) axis, thereby facilitate the recovery of physiological homeostasis following stressful stimuli (e.g., Evans, Sun, McGregor, & Connor, 2012; cf. S. Cacioppo, Capitanio, & Cacioppo, 2014); or 3) oxytocin, a neuropeptide.

For instance, fluoxetine, an antidepressant of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) class, has a broad range of effects including (but not restricted to) improving the behavioral effects of social isolation, anxiety-like behavior and fear responses (Pinna, 2010; Mayo-Wilson et al., 2014). For patients who decline psychological interventions (such as CBT), SSRIs show consistent evidence of improvement. Interestingly, this improvement does not occur through the inhibition of selective serotonin reuptake (as in depression), but rather through elevated cortico-limbic levels of allopregnanolone (ALLO) and BDNF mRNA expression (Pinna, 2010).

Research supporting the hypothesis of a crucial role of ALLO in social isolation demonstrates that: i) the exaggerated contextual fear response expressed by socially-isolated mice can be normalized with a single injection of ALLO (Pibiri, Nelson, Guidotti, Costa, & Pinna, 2008); ii) HPA dysfunction and impairment of hippocampal neurogenesis respectively can be normalized or prevented with the administration of exogenous ALLO either during or following a period of chronic stress; iii) the establishment of depressive/anxiety-like behaviors in rats can be precluded also with administration of exogenous ALLO (Evans et al., 2012; S. Cacioppo, Capitanio, & Cacioppo, 2014; Nelson & Pinna, 2011; Pinna, 2010); and iv) contextual fear conditioning and aggression can be regulated with ALLO (Nelson & Pinna, 2011). Although further investigations of the effects of ALLO on social isolation are needed in humans, ALLO may provide an adjunctive therapeutic target early in cognitive behavioral interventions to alleviate chronic loneliness.

A third potential adjunctive pharmacological treatment for loneliness is oxytocin, a hypothalamic neuropeptide known to have a high sensitivity to social affiliation (Carter et al., 2008; Goossens et al., in this issue; Grippo, 2009; Young et al., 2014). Recent work with prairie voles raises the possibility that oxytocin may help buffer the deleterious neural, behavioral, immune and autonomic effects of social isolation from a pair-bonded partner (Grippo et al., 2009). The prairie vole is a monogamous rodent whose social structure has similarities to that of humans. In prairie voles, long-term social isolation from a mate or partner produces several negative behavioral and physiological alterations, including depressive and anxiety-relevant behaviors, and autonomic and cardiac dysfunction (Grippo et al., 2007ac, 2008, 2011, 2012; McNeal et al., 2014). The exogenous peripheral administration of oxytocin eliminates the adverse behavioral and autonomic changes associated with social isolation in the prairie vole (Grippo et al., 2009, 2012).

Oxytocin administration in humans has been shown to promote pro-social behaviors, affiliation, and trust (Kosfeld et al., 2005), cooperation with others (Declerck, Boone, & Kiyonari, 2014), social synchrony (Arueti et al., 2013), autonomic cardiac control (Norman, Cacioppo et al., 2011a), and to decrease the emotional arousal in response to threatening human stimuli (Norman, Cacioppo et al., 2011b), but negative and inconsistent social effects have also been observed (see Bethlehem et al., 2014, and Bali & Jaggi, 2014, for reviews). For instance, some research studies suggest that oxytocin may make neurologically healthy individuals evaluate participants as more trusting and more pro-social in relaxed social situations and more aggressive in tense social situations (for review see: Bartz, Zaki, Bolger, & Ochsner, 2011), whereas others indicate that oxytocin administration increases outgroup aggression (e.g., De Dreu et al., 2010; Taylor et al., 2006). The fact that oxytocin has some prosocial effects, at least for some individuals or situations, is intriguing but how precisely oxytocin might prove helpful in the treatment of chronic loneliness requires additional research.

 Conclusion 

The etiology of loneliness and its consequences are complex. When the 1978 Task Panel report to the US President’s Commission on Mental Health emphasized the importance of improving health care and easing the pain of those suffering from loneliness, few would have thought that their recommendation would be even more relevant and important today. With increasing evidence that loneliness is a risk factor for mental and physical health problems, attention has begun to turn to interventions for addressing chronic loneliness.

As a first step, there is a need for increased public awareness – and awareness among healthcare providers – that loneliness is a condition that, like chronic pain, can become an affliction for almost anyone. Even popular and high status individuals can find themselves feeling lonely, and the stigma of loneliness further complicates assessment and treatment. Despite the fact that loneliness is a common emotional distress syndrome with a high risk factor for early mortality and a broad variety of physical health and psychiatric issues, it still gets little attention in medical training or in healthcare more generally.

In Masi et al.’s meta-analysis (2011), we identified a need for well-controlled randomized studies focusing on the rehabilitation of the underlying maladaptive social cognition. With the advances made over the past 5 years in the identification of the psychobiological and pharmaceutical mechanisms associated with loneliness and maladaptive social cognition, it may soon be possible to combine (social) cognitive behavioral interventions with short-term adjunctive pharmacological treatments in order to reduce the prevalence of loneliness and its harmful consequences.

 

Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2011 Aug; 15(3)

A Meta-Analysis of Interventions to Reduce Loneliness

Christopher M. Masi,1,3Hsi-Yuan Chen,2,3Louise C. Hawkley,2,3 and John T. Cacioppo2,3

Abstract

Social and demographic trends are placing an increasing number of adults at risk for loneliness, an established risk factor for physical and mental illness. The growing costs of loneliness have led to a number of loneliness reduction interventions. Qualitative reviews have identified four primary intervention strategies: 1) improving social skills, 2) enhancing social support, 3) increasing opportunities for social contact, and 4) addressing maladaptive social cognition. An integrative meta-analysis of loneliness reduction interventions was conducted to quantify the effects of each strategy and to examine the potential role of moderator variables. Results revealed that single group pre-post and non-randomized comparison studies yielded larger mean effect sizes relative to randomized comparison studies. Among studies that used the latter design, the most successful interventions addressed maladaptive social cognition. This is consistent with current theories regarding loneliness and its etiology. Theoretical and methodological issues associated with designing new loneliness reduction interventions are discussed.

This article mentions saying 'hi,' going back to the basics of talking to strangers and using eye contact.  I remember reading on heartiste's blog something to the effect of, whoever is best at striking up conversation with strangers will be most successful with the ladies.  Situational openers, not to be underestimated.  my favorite non-situational openers are joe delaney's (see youtube) and the one about the floppy disc and hard drive.
Read heariste, rollo, knockoff blogs; watch big daws, adrian gee, limitless exp, dating coach channels.  also... any time you encounter something unfamiliar, plug it into google.  arm yourself with information.  little by little, day by day, protracted effort is sure to develop good character.

 

And just keeping a positive attitude, as the study points out, has proven benefits on your social apparatus.  Keep your head up, positivity can also be a vicious cycle.  I know it's hard, especially when you're a shy, lazy bastard who prefers to order dates from the comfort of his couch.  online dating can really reinforce negative thinking, largely because the b*tches think they can get away with saying anything.  it's not true, you will be reported and banned.  but that's a detail.  they also get hit up by a ton of cool dudes and youre competing for attention against their bitchiness.  you just have to roll with it, appreciate their realness, but don't get sucked into their frame.  never volunteer information (it releases tension and makes you look like your boring self, any serious question you answer playfully.. youre john smith, a moderately attractive white male, her next boyfriend, a professional princess, above facebook, 600 lbs and missing teeth but still cute, you dont have friends just a bunch of statues in the basement, etc etc), always make light of things, twist her words, roll with the punches, have prepared responses.  as for the actual conversation joke and tease a bit, if she doesn't ask what youre up to & whether you got your own place, grab her number (do that soon, waiting for her to invite herself rarely pays off).  text her a bit, if she's still stubborn, call.  if you muck up your phonesmanship, next and try again in 6 months.  after 6 months of silence, introduce yourself the same way: john smith, a moderately attractive white male, or your next boyfriend.  also, leave your profile blank and take good pictures, preferably involving hobbies or cool stuff. squinch your eyes a bit and push your forehead and chin forward to accentuate your jawline for the camera.  i think pics with a lot of girls can backfire, as can ones with a lot of people (when you're clearly not doing something cool) or shirtless ones (only if theyre distasteful or try-too-hard, examples: either showcase a hobby, or take it at the beach / working outside)



#13 PeaceAndProsperity

  • Guest
  • 1,194 posts
  • -195
  • Location:Heaven

Posted 18 October 2016 - 05:40 AM

Btw I've tried the "go out and engage people" strategy to "cure" this issue but it's never worked. It truly is just a chemical problem and just as when I forget to take p5p and I find it extremely uncomfortable and threatening when someone looks in my eyes, it's the same with socializing, there's some chemical missing that self-delusion won't change anything for. GABA is one thing, serotonin is another, but I think there're more things. 

 



#14 gamesguru

  • Guest
  • 3,467 posts
  • 429
  • Location:coffeelake.intel.int

Posted 18 October 2016 - 03:00 PM

Exposure is only one leg of the triangle, compliment it with pharms and studying at home.

Schizoid is characterized by more anhedonia, less anxiety. Anxiety responds well to exposure. You get detained by university cops or bounced out of enough bars, you sort of face your fears. Granted, you also carry that negative esteem with you, like no way, aww fuck, those b*tches seriously called security? Fml. But that's a detail.

Andhedonia is harder to treat, just have to soldier thru it; I think it's dopamine and glutamate specific. GABA and serotonin would make the puzzle more complicated. Haven't found many herbal candidates (just the standard ones, which aren't trrrilbly effective), anhedonia is a tough nut to crack. But you don't have to smile, make eye contact or be as enthusiastic as other guys; be a man of few words, be interesting and a bit funny, tell a couple canned stories and give at least some indication you're listening. There's a market for unconventional dudes.
  • Well Written x 1
  • like x 1

sponsored ad

  • Advert
Advertisements help to support the work of this non-profit organisation. To go ad-free join as a Member.

#15 Jedi_Nights

  • Topic Starter
  • Guest
  • 5 posts
  • 7
  • Location:MI
  • NO

Posted 19 October 2016 - 12:52 PM

Its a lot to take in. Thanks guys.

but my main problems are still with anhedonia and variable mood (self-identity too). Do you think this reponds well to therapy? The anhedonia makes job interviews tough, enthusiasm is expected. The variable mood makes friendship hard and also keeping a job, no one likes a spazz. Pretty sure I have a good bit of the 'wanting' anhedonia. Just don't care for stuff, but I'm not depressed. Didnt notice sexual anhedonia, unless it happens too often in a short time, then it loses effect.





Also tagged with one or more of these keywords: outcome, treatment, strategy

1 user(s) are reading this topic

0 members, 1 guests, 0 anonymous users