• Log in with Facebook Log in with Twitter Log In with Google      Sign In    
  • Create Account
  LongeCity
              Advocacy & Research for Unlimited Lifespans

Photo
- - - - -

Cognitive Training Helps Seniors Keep Mental Snap


  • Please log in to reply
1 reply to this topic

#1 doug123

  • Guest
  • 2,424 posts
  • -1
  • Location:Nowhere

Posted 20 December 2006 - 06:15 PM


News source

Cognitive Training Helps Seniors Keep Mental Snap and Crackle

By Judith Groch, MedPage Today Staff Writer
Reviewed by Zalman S. Agus, MD; Emeritus Professor at the University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine.
December 19, 2006

MedPage Today Action Points

o Explain to healthy older adults that training in using their thinking skills and memory -- or even getting mental exercise on their own -- may help forestall mental decline as they grow older.

Review

STATE COLLEGE, Pa., Dec. 19 -- Use it or lose it applies to the brain, according to a study of more than 2,000 persons of Medicare-age living independently in the community.

Mental exercise for high-functioning seniors slowed the expected decline in their thinking ability even five years after a brief intervention, researchers here reported.

However, the effect of cognitive training on functional skills -- the ability to handle everyday tasks -- was less clear and less compelling, according to a report in the Dec. 20 issue of the Journal of the American Medical Association.

Compared with an untreated control group, cognitive training resulted in improved cognitive abilities specific to the abilities trained that continued five years after the initiation, said Sherry Willis, Ph.D., of Pennsylvania State University here, and colleagues.


But when it came to the failure of cognitive training to transfer to activities of daily living, it is possible, Dr. Willis said, that the full extent of cognitive training on everyday activities would take longer than five years in a population that was highly functioning at enrollment.

The controlled single-blind trial recruited 2,832 adults in six U.S. cities (mean age, 73.6; 26% black) who lived independently and had normal cognitive and functional status at the start of the study. Participants were randomly assigned to three intervention groups and to a control group.

The Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly (ACTIVE) study was conducted from April 1998 to December 2004, including a five-year follow-up for 67% of the sample.

The interventions over five to six weeks comprised 10 training sessions (60 to 75 minutes each) for memory (verbal episodic memory), reasoning (inductive reasoning), or speed of processing (visual search and identification). This was followed by four-sessions of booster training at 11 and 35 months in a random sample of those who completed training.

For example, the reasoning group learned ways to remember word lists and sequences of items ideas. The reasoning group practiced pattern recognition, while the processing-speed group learned way to identify an object on a computer screen at increasingly brief exposures.

Immediately after the initial training, 87% of the speed-training group, 74% of the reasoning group, and 26% of the memory group showed improvement in their skills. After five years, the groups performed better on their tests than did those in the control group. The reasoning-training and speed-training groups given booster training benefited the most, the researchers found.

Each intervention maintained the improvement on its specific cognitive ability through five years (memory: effect size, 0.23 [99% CI, 0.11-0.35]; reasoning: effect size, 0.26 [99% CI, 0.17-0.35]; speed of processing: effect size, 0.76 [99% CI, 0.62-0.90]).

Booster training produced additional improvement for reasoning performance (effect size, 0.28; 99% CI, 0.12-0.43) and speed of processing performance (effect size, 0.85; 99% CI, 0.61-1.09).

These improvements, Dr. Willis said, roughly counteract the decline in cognitive performance that would be expected over a seven-to-14 year period in individuals without dementia.

On the other hand, the five-year results of the ACTIVE study provided only limited evidence that cognitive interventions can reduce age-related decline in the activities of daily living. The loss of the ability to perform daily household tasks, manage money, and read medicine-dosing instructions is associated with increased use of hospital, nursing home, and home health services, the researchers said.

Although after five years the intervention participants reported less difficulty than members of the control group in these daily activities, the statistics said otherwise.

At five years, self-reported and performance-based measures of daily function and cognitive abilities were as follows:

The reasoning group reported significantly less difficulty in the instrumental activities of daily living than the control group (effect size, 0.29; 99% confidence interval, 0.03-0.55).
Speed of processing training (effect size, 0.26; 99% CI, −0.002 to 0.51) had no significant effect on daily living function.
Memory training (effect size, 0.20; 99% CI, −0.06 to 0.46) had no significant effect on activities of daily living.

The booster training for the speed of processing group, but not for the two other groups, showed a significant effect on the performance-based functional measure of everyday speed of processing (effect size, 0.30; 99% CI, 0.08-0.52). This may reflect, a need for larger doses of training before effects can be observed in this more cognitively demanding outcome, the researchers said.

Across all the outcomes, evidence for transfer of cognitive training to function was modest and transfer was not observed until the five-year follow-up, the researchers said.

Prior research, they said, has suggested a temporal lag between the onset of cognitive decline and its subsequent impact on daily function, perhaps because of resulting differences in adapting to emerging physical limitations affecting tasks of daily living.

Another possibility is that these delayed effects on function may be attributed to the advantaged nature of the ACTIVE sample, inasmuch as adults with suspected cognitive issues were excluded from the study. Only after the onset of decline in the control groups could the effect of training on function be observed in the intervention group.

"We consider these results promising and support future research to examine if these and other cognitive interventions can prevent or delay functional disability in an aging population," the researchers concluded.

In an accompanying editorial, Sally Shumaker, Ph.D., of Wake Forest University in Winston-Salem, N.C., and coauthors said that the lack of supporting evidence for this "well-designed" study is disappointing and discussed why the effect on the subjects' everyday function was not supported by the data.

It is possible, they said, that the original selection of individuals with relatively high cognitive and physical function might have contributed to this failure. They further suggested that the domain-specific categories used in the study may have had limited the potential for determining an effect on functional status.

Results from the ACTIVE study have several clinical implications, they wrote. If cognitive training could be extended to individuals with Alzheimer's disease, individuals who cannot tolerate existing pharmacological agents would have additional treatment options.

If these programs were standardized and developed for mass market application, they might be made available to seniors through nonhealth-care facilities (schools, churches) as well as through health-care facilities. This would give individuals a greater sense of control over the disturbing prospect of cognitive decline.

Summing up, the editorial writers said, "addressing the growing threat of an aging population with limited options for maintaining cognitive function is a major challenge, and multiple promising avenues merit exploration."

Karlene Ball, Ph.D., a coauthor the Penn State study, reported that she owns stock in VisualAwareness Inc, which owns the patent for the Useful Field of View testing and training software. No other authors reported disclosures.

Primary source: Journal of the American Medical Association
Source reference:
Willis SL, et al "Long-term Effects of Cognitive Training on Everyday Functional Outcomes in Older Adults" JAMA 2006; 296: 2806-2814.

Additional source: Journal of the American Medical Association
Source reference:
Shumaker SA, et al "Behavior-Based Interventions to Enhance Cognitive Functioning and Independence in Older Adults" JAMA 2006; 296: 2852-2854.

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------
Disclaimer
The information presented in this activity is that of the authors and does not necessarily represent the views of the University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine, MedPage Today, and the commercial supporter. Specific medicines discussed in this activity may not yet be approved by the FDA for the use as indicated by the writer or reviewer. Before prescribing any medication, we advise you to review the complete prescribing information, including indications, contraindications, warnings, precautions, and adverse effects. Specific patient care decisions are the responsibility of the healthcare professional caring for the patient. Please review our Terms of Use.



© 2004-2006 MedPage Today, LLC. All Rights Reserved.

#2 doug123

  • Topic Starter
  • Guest
  • 2,424 posts
  • -1
  • Location:Nowhere

Posted 12 January 2007 - 09:39 PM

News Source: Science Daily

Posted Image

Source: Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care

Date: January 12, 2007
More on: Dementia, Alzheimer's Research, Alzheimer's, Intelligence, Language Acquisition, Multiple Sclerosis Research

Bilingualism Has Protective Effect In Delaying Onset Of Dementia By Four Years, Canadian Study Shows

Science Daily — Canadian scientists have found astonishing evidence that the lifelong use of two languages can help delay the onset of dementia symptoms by four years compared to people who are monolingual.

There has been much interest and growing scientific literature examining how lifestyle factors such as physical activity, education and social engagement may help build "cognitive reserve" in later years of life. Cognitive reserve refers to enhanced neural plasticity, compensatory use of alternative brain regions, and enriched brain vasculature, all of which are thought to provide a general protective function against the onset of dementia symptoms.

Now scientists with the Rotman Research Institute at the Baycrest Research Centre for Aging and the Brain have found the first evidence that another lifestyle factor, bilingualism, may help delay dementia symptoms. The study is published in the February 2007 issue of Neuropsychologia (Vol.45, No.2).


"We are pretty dazzled by the results," says principal investigator Ellen Bialystok, Ph.D., whose research team at Baycrest included psychologist Dr. Fergus Craik, a world authority on age-related changes in memory processes, and neurologist Dr. Morris Freedman, an eminent authority on understanding the mechanisms underlying cognitive impairment due to diseases such as Alzheimer's.

"Our study found that speaking two languages throughout one's life appears to be associated with a delay in the onset of symptoms of dementia by four years compared to those who speak one language," says Dr. Bialystok, Professor of Psychology at York University and Associate Scientist at the Rotman Research Institute at Baycrest.

The study follows on the heels of previous published reports by Dr. Bialystok and colleagues showing that bilingualism enhances attention and cognitive control in both children and older adults. Those results inspired Bialystok and her research team to ask, "So what does this mean for the onset of dementia?"

In this present study, researchers set out to answer that question by examining the diagnostic records of 184 patients who came to Baycrest's Sam and Ida Ross Memory Clinic between 2002 and 2005 with cognitive complaints. Of that group, 91 were monolingual and 93 were bilingual. The bilinguals included speakers of 25 different languages, the most prevalent being Polish, Yiddish, German, Romanian and Hungarian.

Researchers found that 132 patients met criteria for probable Alzheimer's; the remaining 52 were diagnosed with other dementias. Patient data included Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores (a measure of general cognitive functioning), years of education and occupation. The MMSE scores were equivalent for the monolingual and bilingual groups at their initial visit to the clinic, indicating comparable levels of impairment. The age of onset of cognitive impairment was determined by the interviewing neurologist at the first clinic visit who asked patients and their families or caregivers when symptoms were first noticed.

The researchers determined that the mean age of onset of dementia symptoms in the monolingual group was 71.4 years, while the bilingual group was 75.5 years. This difference remained even after considering the possible effect of cultural differences, immigration, formal education, employment and even gender as influencers in the results.

"There are no pharmacological interventions that are this dramatic," says Dr. Freedman, who is Head of the Division of Neurology, and Director of the Memory Clinic at Baycrest, referring to the four-year delay in onset of symptoms for bilingual patients.

"The data show a huge protective effect," adds co-investigator Dr. Craik, who cautioned that this is still a preliminary finding but nonetheless in line with a number of other recent findings about lifestyle effects on dementia.

The team is working on a follow-up study that will further examine bilingualism and dementia onset. They plan to conduct interviews and cognitive assessments on bilingual and monolingual patients in Baycrest's Memory Clinic and follow them for a few years.

The study in Neuropsychologia was funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research. Baycrest is an internationally renowned academic health sciences centre affiliated with the University of Toronto. Baycrest provides a spectrum of health care services to older adults, and conducts basic and applied research with a strong focus on brain functioning and mental health.

Note: This story has been adapted from a news release issued by Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care.

sponsored ad

  • Advert
Click HERE to rent this advertising spot for BRAIN HEALTH to support LongeCity (this will replace the google ad above).



0 user(s) are reading this topic

0 members, 0 guests, 0 anonymous users